Students’ Metaphors as Descriptors of Effective and Ineffective Learning
Experiences
Karlene Ferrante*
Department of Communication
+1 715 346 3712
Kathryn M. Olson
Department of Communication
University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee
Theresa Castor
Department of Communication
University of Wisconsin-Parkside
Mary Hoeft
Department of Communication
University of
John R. Johnson
Department of Communication
University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee
Renee A. Meyers
Department of Communication
University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee
Abstract
The purpose of this study was to gain a better understanding of how college
students characterise their learning experiences.
A Scholarship of Teaching and Learning research project was undertaken in
which students were asked to
describe, and create
metaphors for, effective and ineffective learning experiences.
Our focus was on experiences explicitly and solely perceived by students
(i.e., listening to their voices) rather than on outcome-based or
performance-based measures. Data
was collected from 142
Key Words:
Scholarship of Teaching and Learning, Learning, Metaphors, Student Perspectives
Introduction
Lee S. Shulman (2000), president of The Carnegie Foundation for the Advancement
of Teaching, called fidelity “to the
learning of students one is committed to teach and serve” a core
professional commitment that must motivate Scholarship of Teaching and Learning
(SoTL) research (pp. 95-96). Moreover,
Shulman (1999) indicated that “to take learning seriously, we need to take
learners seriously” (p. 12; italics
added). One way to take learners
seriously is to ask the learners
themselves about their perceptions of the learning process.
Although some SoTL researchers have focused their investigations squarely
on the student-learning link (see Linkon, 2000; Morehead & Shedd, 1996 as
exemplars), continued work in this domain is needed if we are to better
understand the central role of students in the learning enterprise.
Hence this study examined students’ own perceptions and characterisations of
effective and ineffective learning experiences.
Simple as this task appears on the surface, learning involves complex
sets of emotions, perceptions, implied relationships, and unspoken assumptions
that can be hard for students to recount.
So it is important for researchers investigating students’ voices to
encourage them to reflect, not only on intellectual aspects, but also on the
social and emotional facets of those experiences.
For these reasons, we
thought that metaphors, more so than simple descriptions, might better capture
the nuances of meaning and unspoken assumptions associated with learning.
“Metaphors,”
wrote Eubanks (1999), “not only index those assumptions but they participate in
a complex conversation in which the rhetorical implications of our assumptions
are played out” (p. 196).
Moreover, when producing metaphor, the creator draws upon his/her own
experience, culture, and context to shape the implied comparison between the
dissimilar entities. In this way,
students are free to choose any metaphoric comparisons and can produce rich,
creative, and unique images.
We primarily are concerned with metaphors in the traditional sense of linking a
tenor (a specific learning experience) with a vehicle (the comparison offered by
the student). The tenor is the principal
subject that the vehicle figuratively illuminates; so in “learning is a
three-ring circus,” learning is the tenor and three-ring circus is the vehicle (Franke,
2000; Richards, 1936, pp. 96-100).
However, we offered students the option to use similes, and some used analogies.
In the following sections, we describe how we used metaphor to elicit student
voices regarding effective and ineffective learning experiences.
Our focus was on
experiences explicitly and solely perceived by students (i.e., listening to
their voices) rather than on outcome-based or performance-based measures.
We first provide a conceptual
framework for our project by identifying the significance of metaphor for
understanding the learning process. We then provide the details of our research
study and elaborate on the metaphoric themes that were generated from the
students’ experiences. Three themes for effective/ineffective learning were
discovered in the metaphors: connection/disconnection,
empowerment/disempowerment, and engagement/disengagement.
We conclude with a discussion of the
implications of our findings for teaching and learning and for the role of
metaphor as a tool to illicit student perceptions in SoTL research.
Metaphor as a
Conceptual Framework for Investigating Student Voices
Metaphors have long been studied as “an essential ingredient of communication
and consequently of great educational value” (Ortony, 1975, p. 45).
Ortony argued, “[M]etaphors, and their close relatives, similes and
analogies, have been used as teaching devices since the earliest writings of
civilised man [sic]” (p. 45).
Publications that treat metaphor as a teaching tool or philosophical perspective
on learning are plentiful (e.g., Cameron, 2003; Gossi, 1999; Taylor, 1984).
Academics have also used metaphor to characterise various teaching styles
and to extend the implications of teaching styles for education and student
motivation (Grasha, 1996). In addition,
metaphors have been used to describe students’ learning styles (Grasha, 1990),
as well as the general experiences of being a freshman, being in college, or
attending a particular type of higher educational institution (Jorgensen-Earp
&
Staton, 1993; Lattin, Kerssen-Griep, & Thede, 2002; McMillan & Cheney, 1996).
For the most part, these past studies have focused on general characteristics of
teaching and learning (e.g., teacher and student roles; students’ identities and
cultural characteristics; the freshman experience; perceptions of grades and
classroom environments) rather than on specific situated instances of perceived
effective and ineffective learning.
As Jorgensen-Earp and Staton (1993) pointed out, research on educational
metaphors has paid little attention to student views, concentrating
disproportionately on administrators and instructors (p. 127).
Yet asking students to metaphorically characterise effective and ineffective
learning experiences may allow for description of complex, organised impressions
that are difficult to articulate in more literal language.
A metaphor "suppresses some details,
emphasises others--in short, organises
our view" (Black, 1962, pp. 39, 41).
Lakoff and Johnson (1980) concluded that metaphors offer particularly
well-rounded insights because they express both logical and non-logical
dimensions through “an imaginative rationality” (p. 193).
The metaphor tells at least as much about the user’s perspective
as about the subject matter.
“Each metaphor,” argued Edelman (1971) “intensifies selected perceptions
and ignores others. Each metaphor
can be a subtle way of highlighting what one wants to believe and avoiding what
one does not wish to face” (p. 67).
Eubanks (1999) stressed the
importance of considering a metaphor in its specific situational context rather
than extracting it from the situation, as many studies of metaphor do.
We implemented such an approach by asking students to recall their own
metaphors for a single specific effective and ineffective learning experience
from a previous class they had taken.
This approach is especially important given Hardcastle, Yamamoto, Parkay,
& Chan’s (1985) finding that researcher-generated metaphors did not always match
student-generated metaphors. Even
more striking was the finding in the Hardcastle et al. (1985) investigation that
the consistency of metaphors describing education was greater among students
(even those from diverse cultures) than it was between researchers and students.
Hence it is important to listen closely to students’ own metaphors regarding
their own specific experiences rather than assume or impose researcher-generated
metaphors.
Relatedly, Grasha (1990) studied
student-generated metaphors of effective and ineffective learning at the
course-level. Although Grasha’s
findings are foundational to this investigation, we question whether the whole
course is the appropriate level for studying students’ perceptions of learning.
Instead, we assume that students’ metaphors will be tied more closely to
specific learning experiences and
that both effective and ineffective learning experiences can occur in a single
course. We reasoned, therefore,
that the complexity of learning warrants a more particularised unit of analysis
such as the single learning experience so that students can more specifically
and comprehensively identify effective and ineffective aspects.
Hence we asked students to describe a specific effective and a specific ineffective learning experience rather than asking them to consider the more general level of whole courses. After providing a detailed example, students were asked to characterise their two chosen experiences using a metaphor or simile. Finally, students were requested to explain why they chose that metaphor or why it was especially descriptive in that case. We reasoned that by using a more contained level of analysis (situated learning experiences); students would provide richer and more detailed descriptions and metaphors. These descriptions and metaphors offered us insight into students’ actual learning experiences in all their complex subjectivity and provided important prescriptions for improving learning experiences for students.
Method
Collection
Procedures
Students enrolled in
classes at four different
Eliciting Metaphors
We chose primarily
open-ended questions for data collection and qualitative methodology for data
analysis. We selected this approach
because we wanted students to have the freedom to express their metaphors and
experiences without any constraints.
In addition, the grounded theory method (described later) provided us
with a way to capture the richness and detail in the students’ descriptions of
learning experiences that we considered essential.
A set of open-ended
questions was used to collect students’ metaphors and perceptions of effective
and ineffective learning experiences.
The first question asked participants to identify a specific effective
learning experience from a college class.
Then the participants were asked to respond to a series of questions
designed to elicit their perceptions of that effective learning experience,
utilising the following prompts:
In similar fashion, a second set of questions asked subjects to identify a specific ineffective learning experience from a college class. The same five prompts were provided and the word “effective” was replaced with “ineffective” (e.g. “Why was this experience ineffective?”). Finally, participants were asked to provide some demographic information (graduate/undergraduate status, major or intended major, number of credits earned to date, years of age, gender, college or university presently attending) which provided frequency data and were therefore analysed quantitatively.
Categorising the
Metaphors
Students’ responses
were placed in a single database. A
grounded theory approach (Annells, 1997; Parry, 1998; Strauss & Corbin, 1990,
1998) was used to analyse the student comments.
Using this approach, data is coded into increasingly comprehensive
aggregates of categories or constructs which are identified based on their
properties or dimensions (Cutcliffe, 2000; Fassinger, 2005).
It involves employing an iterative, constant comparison coding process
until no new themes, categories, or relationships are discovered.
Hence, the first
step in this study was to read all the students’ responses to all of the
questions. Following this reading,
the first four authors began by independently identifying the multiple types of
metaphors provided by the students.
They then each independently developed categories to organise the metaphors
according to the relationships they perceived among and between the metaphors.
Next they met to share their independently generated categories in a
face-to-face session. At this
session, the lists of categories were compared, contrasted, and debated with
respect to the data. The four
authors looked at the relationships among their sets of categories and discarded
or combined any that overlapped.
Through group consensus, a common and manageable set of categories useful for
interpreting the metaphors was generated.
This resulted in a set of seven categories (connections, empowerment,
discovery, satisfaction, disconnection, disempowerment, and disengagement).
As this list of
seven categories indicates, four categories were originally identified for
effective learning experiences metaphors and three separate categories were
identified for ineffective learning experiences metaphors.
However in discussion of the categories and by comparing categories to
each other, it became clear that the metaphors for effective and ineffective
learning were essentially mirror images of each other.
The satisfaction and discovery codes were combined to form an engagement
category. We found that both
satisfaction and discovery metaphors typically described engaging learning
situations (i.e., learning was like:
going to a concert, attending
political discussions, embarking on field trips, visiting a museum, opening a
window into a darkened room, turning on a light bulb). Moreover students
often recounted satisfying experiences
in classes as situations where “you were actively learning.”
So in the end, and
after much discussion, the four authors determined that three sets of
oppositional categories (connection/disconnection, empowerment/ disempowerment,
engagement/disengagement) could be used to code metaphors for both effective and
ineffective learning experiences.
This set of three macro-level categories was then used by the third and fourth
authors again working independently to code a set of metaphors from the data.
These codes were reviewed, compared,
discussed, and modified as necessary by the first four authors.
Following this procedure, the categories were deemed both exhaustive and
valid for this sample, and the remaining data was coded by the first and third
authors. Finally, frequencies were computed for the demographic responses.
Participant
Information
Of the 142 subjects who participated, 36% attended 2-year colleges and 64% attended comprehensive/doctoral universities. In terms of gender, 32% were male and 62% female and 6% did not identify their sex. The average (mean) age was 22.38 years and ranged from 17 to 45 years old. The vast majority (97%) were undergraduates. Forty percent of the participants identified their academic major as Communication. The remaining 60% of the participants indicated a variety of academic majors with no single major from this group having more than 1% of the total. The number of credits earned by the subjects ranged from 0 to 123 with the mean being 49 credits.
Metaphors for
Learning:
Connections/Disconnections
One set of metaphors that students used to describe effective or ineffective
learning experiences were metaphors describing the connections/disconnections
that students saw with their instructors, fellow students, and the subject
matter. In effective situations, students’ metaphors described the classroom and
subject matter as part of a web in which the teacher and students worked
together to forge links between course material and real life.
See Table 1 for representative examples of student metaphors.
Table 1.
Representative Metaphors
Connection |
Empowerment |
Engagement |
Spider’s web |
Acquiring the tools |
Refreshing rain |
Evolution |
Umbrella in a storm |
Breath of fresh air |
Epiphinal flood |
Light switch turned on |
Butterfly emerging from cocoon |
Two peas in a pod |
Wake-up call |
Performance at a play |
Sitting in a coffee shop |
Learning to ride a bicycle |
Visiting a museum |
Big happy family |
Sailing and avoiding a storm |
Arguing with the Pope |
Family dinner table |
Receiving a green light |
Rising from a dusty tomb |
Disconnection |
Disempowerment |
Disengagement |
Headless chicken |
Stripped of a voice |
Watching paint dry |
Ice skater on cement |
Running blind in a marathon |
Sleeping in |
Boring slumber party |
Traveling on a one-way street |
Watching a turtle cross the road |
Robot in assembly line |
Out of control |
Waiting in line |
Talking to a post |
Feeling like drowning |
Sitting on the sidelines |
Drowning in an ocean |
Being in a train wreck |
Broken record |
Dancing around a question |
Crashing into a wall |
Waiting for a phone call |
Some of the metaphors that students used to describe connections with teachers
that contributed to effective learning experiences included describing the
teacher-student connection as an evolution (i.e., a relational development over
the course of the semester), an epiphinal flood (i.e., a strong and immediate
connection with the teacher), and like “Jesus (teacher) going out to find lost
sheep (students).” One student used the metaphor of “two peas in a pod” to
describe her connection with her teacher and noted that
“The way this teacher went about his
class made me tune into him and not the things around me.
… This teacher and I were alike. I understood him and his ways of
teaching.”
Students used metaphors to identify connections with classmates as important to
effective learning experiences as well. For example, they indicated that
effective learning experiences were like sitting in a coffee shop with friends
or being a big happy family. One student used the metaphor of a family dinner
table to explain how the use of small group discussion with other students
helped him feel comfortable and contributed to an effective learning experience.
Another student
identified how connections with other students in a classroom group contributed
to an effective learning experience, indicating that “The teacher was not
involved much, it was more about exploring independently how things work and
learning to work as a team.”
These students’ metaphors and
descriptions of connections among teachers and learners illustrate an implicit
awareness by these students of the importance of community and relationships in
forging effective learning experiences.
On the other end of the scale,
students also produced metaphors
describing ineffective learning experiences that described multiple forms of
disconnection from course content, other students, and the instructor.
Their vivid metaphors included feeling like a “headless chicken” or “an
ice skater on cement” or like attending a “boring slumber party where everyone
just lays about.” Students
disconnected from classes in which they saw no value.
One student, explaining his disconnection from the course content, wrote,
“There was no value in the material for the real world (as I saw it). I did not
try to get anything out of it, therefore I didn’t.”
Another student, describing herself as a robot in an assembly line,
wrote, “I felt detached from the material and I lacked a sense of purpose.”
Students also spoke of their disconnection from instructors. One student described her feelings as follows: “The professor was just lecturing. He was using words that we did not understand…this [a decapitated chicken] is exactly what I felt like.” Still another student indicated that her attempts to communicate with her professor were as futile as trying to talk to a post: “I could not find out the information I needed to know pertaining to his class because he simply would not talk to me.” The emotional disconnection by professors left some students feeling numb and demoralised: “It left the majority of our class in a state of awe that the professor didn’t care that nobody understood the material.” Perhaps one of the more complex metaphoric statements was made by a student who felt as though she was drowning in an ocean: “The teacher danced around what we really wanted to know but never really answered our question…Information kept coming and no matter how hard you tried to understand it, you just couldn’t learn how to swim.”
Metaphors for
Learning:
Empowerment/Disempowerment
In terms of empowerment/disempowerment, metaphors of effective learning
experiences described how acquiring the appropriate tools and guidance allowed
students to accomplish learning tasks.
See Table 1. Many students
recognised that one outcome of an effective learning experience is movement
toward independent thinking. Some
of the metaphors in this category compared effective learning experiences to an
umbrella in a storm, a light switch turned on, a wake-up call, learning to ride
a bicycle, being lit up like light bulbs, sailing on the sea while avoiding the
storm, or receiving a green light. Another student’s metaphor compared the
experience to being asked to try smelly cheese and finding it actually tasted
good. One student explained that his teacher contributed to his effective
learning experience by encouraging discovery:
Though this was a solo project and I did all of my research alone, I still felt
that by making this project a "big deal" the professor was encouraging us to
discover. This experience was very effective because I was allowed to discover
for myself what things truly interested me. . . .
Conversely, student-produced metaphors of ineffective learning experiences
centered on disempowerment, a sense that they had been stripped of a voice in
their education. One student described himself as dirt beneath the feet of his
professor, explaining that “the professor not only called us names, but would
not stop to answer our questions.
He treated us like we were so much lower than him in our intelligence.
I didn’t want to speak out in class at all, because I was afraid he would
degrade me in front of the class.”
Finally, one student characterised an ineffective instructor as an abusive
parent that keeps talking or yelling without waiting for a response.
“It was a horrible experience, and it started my first day of college
when the professor told our class that ‘on average, ½ of all XX students fail
XXX. There are 24 of you in class.
This means that at least 12 of you will fail.’
That was my first day of college.
What a way to start!”
Other students described themselves as feeling out of control. One student compared the helplessness of the experience to drowning or being lost: “For the whole semester I felt like I could hardly keep my head above the water . . . I don’t (sic) feel welcome to ask a lot of questions and I felt lost.” In a similar vein, others indicated their ineffective learning experiences were like crashing into a wall, being in a train wreck, traveling up a creek without a paddle, playing baseball without knowing how, running blind in a marathon, teaching themselves how to dance, climbing uphill wearing greasy, non-traction shoes, or traveling down a one-way street: “It was like hitting a road block in my education.”
Metaphors for
Learning: Engagement/Disengagement
Finally,
many of the metaphors
that were coded in the third category illustrated how engaged learning yielded
moments of discovery (one of the original seven categories that was folded into
this set). In short,
active learning, including experiential
activities, helped students develop new understandings.
See Table 1.
Some described engaged learning experiences as being
like a refreshing rain, a fresh breeze, or a breath of fresh air.
One student described an active learning experience as being similar to a
butterfly emerging from its cocoon. Another student recounted how a
biology class fieldtrip felt like “going on a vacation” because “you actually
got to see the creatures alive and not in a glass jar in a lab,” a comment
emphasising the cross-over with recognising the material’s relevance to the
“real world”. Another student described how an effective physics class
was like a performance at a play when the teacher “was demonstrating voltage and
to help us understand he placed a paper bag on his head with a picture of a
light bulb on it.”
Other students’ metaphors described engaged learning as:
going to a concert, attending public forums or political discussions,
embarking on field trips, visiting a museum, viewing a beautiful painting,
reading a good book, opening a window into a darkened room, turning on a light
bulb, and going on archaeological digs.
One student described an effective learning experience as similar to an
AA meeting explaining how peer feedback engaged the students in a discussion
that allowed them to discover that “all the other students and even the grad
student had similar views and opinions” This
comment, too, suggests a cross-over with the importance of peer relationships in
effective learning.
Some students focused on spiritual or growth metaphors to describe engaging
learning experiences. One described
it as being like “arguing with the Pope;” another student suggested it was like
rising from a dusty tomb to dancing spirits, yet another identified it as a
slice of heaven. One student
recounted a satisfying experience in a class that used a great deal of
discussion as a way to grow in her learning: “You didn't just sit back and
listen to someone talk, you were actively learning.”
A first year student explained how her teacher used various in-class
exercises to get students comfortable with discussing:
It was like “going back to kindergarten and being able to be open to new
ideas without prejudice.”
Alternatively,
metaphors used by students to describe disengagement and boredom included
watching paint dry, sleeping in, waiting for that phone call that never comes,
watching a turtle cross the road, waiting in line, and taking a field trip to a
pencil factory (i.e., “It sounds good in principle, but actually being there is
boring and a waste of time”). A
freshman compared her ineffective learning experience to her junior prom:
“Going …with the hopes of having a wonderful time and sitting on the
sideline watching the disco ball go round and round while everyone looked at
it…I felt like it was something I really wanted to experience but gained nothing
from it.” One student viewed the
ineffective learning experience as a broken record, noting that “it got
redundant. It was doing the same
thing every week. I guess I get
bored with the same thing and like new things and ideas.”
Reflective summary
In general, we found students’ metaphors of both effective and ineffective
learning experiences to be complex, informative, and richly descriptive.
There were wonderful pictures of engaged, connected, and empowered
learning environments in these data.
Unfortunately, the metaphors of ineffective learning experiences were
more painfully vivid and emotionally charged than those that described effective
learning experiences. Is it perhaps
the case that students recall, and remember, their ineffective learning
experiences more vividly than those experiences that are more effective?
Our data hint that this may be the case as students’ ineffective learning
metaphors recalled disconnections from teachers, course material, and
classmates. Students felt
disempowered and without tools to succeed. They felt disengaged and victimised.
These experiences seem to leave an imprint just as powerful as--maybe more
powerful than--the satisfaction of the effective experiences described by the
same students. If that is indeed
the case, these findings have important implications for retention programs,
student success initiatives, and the coaching we give new students on how to
interpret the impact of their classroom experiences.
Implications
Overview of Findings
Three categories of
effective/ineffective learning emerged from our analysis of the student
generated metaphors: (a)
connection/disconnection, (b) empowerment/disempowerment, and (c)
engagement/disengagement. The
students’ metaphors described effective learning experiences that included
connections with professors and fellow students, empowerment to discover and
learn independently, and engagement in learning.
Conversely, student metaphors painted ineffective learning experiences
as places of disconnection between instructors, other students, and the course
material. They resented instructors
who merely took them along for a ride, and they rejected course material that
they viewed as boring and irrelevant to their lives.
The implications of these metaphors for instructional practice are
explored next, as well as how metaphor analysis might be fruitfully used by
instructors to listen more closely to student voices in SoTL research.
Implications for
Instructional Practice: Connection
Students in this
study perceived a clear difference in connections between effective and
ineffective learning situations. These
results urge us to build connection into our classes to promote learning.
We can turn to past research on teaching and learning for examples of how
to accomplish this. The vast body
of research on group-centered learning provides strong support for the role of
student-to- student connection for effective learning.
Research shows that
group-centered learning develops students’ communication, conflict management,
and problem solving skills (Colbeck, Campbell, & Bjorklund, 2000; Herbster &
Hannula, 1992), increases liking among students (Slavin, 1991), improves
students’ self-esteem (Johnson, Johnson & Taylor, 1993), and promotes
interaction among diverse student populations (Johnson & Johnson, 1981).
Perhaps most importantly, in post-secondary education research, studies
have shown a consistently positive relationship between group-centered learning
and student achievement (Considine, Meyers, & Timmerman, 2006; Doran, Sullivan,
& Klein, 1993; Felder, Felder, & Diets, 1998; Herbster & Hannula, 1992; Tlusty,
McIntyre, & Eierman, 1993).
Moreover, there is a large body of
research demonstrating that the more immediate (or highly connected) a teacher
is to students, the more likely students will be motivated to learn (Witt,
Wheeless, & Allen, 2006). Immediacy
describes interaction behaviors that produce a perception of physical or
psychological closeness. A recent
meta-analysis (Witt et al.) showed a meaningful relationship between teacher
immediacy behaviors (verbal or nonverbal) and overall student learning.
As verbal and nonverbal immediacy increase on the part of the instructor,
affective learning meaningfully improves.
In addition, students like more highly immediate (connected) instructors
and perceive that they learn more in their courses (p. 161).
This perceived learning may well translate into greater motivation to
keep learning, to complete a program of study, and remain in college.
The importance of connection not
only applies to the instructor-student relationship in the classroom, but more
recently, research has demonstrated that out-of-classroom communication (OCC)
connections between students and teachers (e.g., email, office visits, telephone
calls) is vital to effective learning experiences (Aylor & Oppliger, 2003;
Jones, 2008). Researchers have
discovered that out-of-class communication has a direct and positive influence
on students’ academic performance (e.g. Pascarella, 1980; Spady, 1970;
Theophilides & Terensini, 1981). These studies find that more out-of-classroom
interaction between students and instructors leads to greater educational
aspirations as well as improved grade point averages (Nadler & Nadler, 2001).
In addition, past research has reported a large number of additional
benefits to students who engage in more OCC with faculty members. These benefits
include improved academic and cognitive development (Terensini, Pascarella &
Blimling, 1996), better developed career plans (Pascarella, 1980), higher
educational aspirations ( Pascarella & Terensini, 1991), greater levels of
academic integration into the university (Milem & Berger, 1997), more
satisfaction with college experiences (Astin, 1977; Pascarella, 1980), better
intellectual and personal development (Pascarella, 1980), and increased feelings
of affirmation, confidence, and self-worth (Kuh, 1995).
Certainly data from this
investigation, and past research, make a strong case for connection within, and
outside, the classroom. This may be
even truer today than it was a decade ago because our current traditional
students (often called millennials) are especially interested in
student-to-student and student-to-faculty linkages.
For this generation, connectivity is ubiquitous, and living in a fully
connected world means that millennials participate in real-time conversations at
any time, in any place, with anybody.
Additionally they are used to spending time in groups, real or virtual
(Howe & Struass, 2000; Lancaster & Stillman, 2002).
Many millennials like collective action and feel less pressured
individually when they are working with a group.
In short, they often want to be part of learning communities, with hubs
and spokes of learning, rather than a one-size-fits-all approach (Frand, 2000).
Those expectations provide a setting ripe for continued development of
connected classrooms and learning experiences.
In terms of specific instructional
practices, these findings strongly encourage us to find ways to make connections
both within and outside the classroom.
How can we best accomplish that? Simple
efforts like arriving at class early to talk with students informally before
class, encouraging students to visit in your office, responding to students’
email, setting up classroom learning activities that promote student connections
(dyadic interactions, small group experiences, whole class discussions, etc.)
all stimulate and encourage connections between students as well as between
students and instructors. Each
instructor has a unique way of connecting with students through verbal,
nonverbal, or written channels. It
seems that the form for establishing connection is not nearly as important as is
the practice of connecting itself.
Implications for
Instructional Practice: Empowerment
The student voices
in the second category of metaphors on empowerment identified discovery and
independent thinking as important for effective learning experiences.
What does this mean for teaching and learning in our classrooms?
Although most
instructors believe (and much of the literature argues) that clarity and
transparency is vital to effective instruction (McCroskey,
Theories of ambiguity and paradox
suggest that we live in a world of “both/and” rather than “either/or”.
The tension between clarity and ambiguity is inherent and ubiquitous and
cannot be eliminated. Therefore, it
must be managed. How might this
tension be managed in our classrooms to empower students and motivate learning?
The idea of strategic ambiguity
which is the purposeful use of ambiguity by instructors to orient toward
multiple goals to help students learn may prove useful here (see Hufford, 1966;
Eisenberg, 1984; Olson, 2001).
Clarity and ambiguity must co-exist in the classroom in such a way that students
feel secure enough (buoyed by clear instructions and explanations) to take risks
in learning that moves them toward independence (to grapple with the ambiguity).
In other words, clarity is important as
a foundation for learning, but ambiguity offers the greatest potential for deep
understanding. The discomfort and
disorientation that accompany strategic ambiguity (when framed by clear
expectations and explanation) may enable students to find their own voices.
What does this finding suggest for
instructional practice? We think it
emboldens us to build puzzles and mysteries and dilemmas into our classroom
learning activities. That is, instead of
providing students with comprehensive parameters for an assignment, we might
instead establish a general framework of acceptance that allows students to
create, imagine, and produce their own projects within those more general
guidelines. We can move away from
instructional practices that tell students exactly what they need to do to get
an “A” toward a culture where they
discover what it is they need to do to get an “A.”
This may be a difficult struggle and transition for students as well as
instructors, but our data suggest that students favor discovery and independent
thinking. If so, as instructors, we
might best encourage those characteristics by providing less structured
certainty and more puzzles as we construct learning activities and assignments.
Implications for
Instructional Practice: Engagement
Although there are
many contributing factors that constitute effective and ineffective learning
situations (i.e., learning styles of students, previous learning experiences,
students’ abilities to adapt, the learning environment, assessment methods,
instructor enthusiasm, motivation, among multiple other factors), the results of
this study show that students found classes in which they were actively engaged
to be better learning experiences overall.
Engaging students is no small task.
Many of us experiment with a variety of educational strategies or
techniques to accomplish this task.
The results of this study suggest those efforts are appropriate.
Interestingly, none
of the student-generated metaphors revealed any single type of educational
strategy or technique as inherently more or less engaging.
Our data, which cut across different
courses, disciplines, and instructors, indicated that many different
instructional strategies or methods were viewed as engaging or disengaging.
Some respondents’ effective learning experiences centered on lectures
while others found lectures very disengaging.
Others described effective learning experiences that involved groups or
active learning, while other respondents’ ineffective learning experiences
centered on those very same instructional methods.
Students did not consistently
identify single instructional strategies as either engaging or disengaging.
Students from the same class sometimes found the very same version of an
activity engaging while others found it to be less so.
Perhaps instructional strategies or methods that match (or fail to match)
individuals’ learning styles or preferences may explain these differences.
But the good news for instructors seems to be that it is not the
instructional method alone that influences student perceptions of effective and
ineffective learning experiences.
That is, a lecture, a small group discussion, or a hands-on activity can all be
engaging or disengaging, depending on the learner, so employing a mixture of
learning strategies is likely the best way to reach the most students.
Finally, the students’
engagement/disengagement metaphors that related to satisfaction or
dissatisfaction with the course should not necessarily determine the shape or
content of a course. Blunsdon,
Reed, McNeil, and McEachern (2003) found that students’ level of enjoyment
dominated their short-term evaluations of learning (i.e., students who enjoyed
an experience believed that they had learned more) as well as outcomes such as
expressed willingness to enroll in a related course; yet it did not necessarily
lead to longer-term changes in cognitive ability or improvement on
performance-based learning assessments (also see Miller, Wilkes, Cheetham, &
Goodwin, 1993). Abbott (2006)
discovered that providing certain learning materials or experiences that
increased individuals’ satisfaction (as measured by a decrease in student
complaints or positive student feedback) did not improve student learning (as
measured by exam scores). In the
end, more research is needed to determine exactly what it is that makes any
instructional method effective or ineffective for different students.
It may well have to do with the presence (or absence) of the previously
mentioned experiences of connection, empowerment, and engagement.
Metaphors as Method
This study also
demonstrates that situated, student-generated metaphor may be a valuable tool
for understanding how students conceptualise their role in the learning process.
As a general rule, teachers are not in the habit of probing the
subjective experiences of students.
Yet students in this investigation were able to offer quite sophisticated
metaphors for describing their learning experiences.
In addition, as
teachers we may need to clarify and articulate for our students our own
metaphors and expectations for effective and ineffective learning.
A mismatch between the metaphors of teachers and learners can result in
learning experiences that fail to meet learners’ and teachers’ expectations (Mills,
Ayre, Hands, & Carden, 2005). Mills
et al. concluded that faculty misperceptions of students learning styles, for
example, contributed to an active conflict between instructors’ well-meaning
teaching choices and their students’ optimal learning.
Such mismatches can affect students’ motivation and interest and may
result in attrition. Sharing with and
listening to each others’ metaphors for effective and ineffective learning
promotes understanding that may help improve all parties’ experiences.
In like manner, differences in
instructors’ and students’ metaphors of effective learning situations could
result in instructors selecting strategies that fail to recognise their
students’ expectations. For
example, some students do not perceive lectures as connecting, engaging, or
empowering. Certainly lectures have
the capacity to be all of these things, but compared to many other active
learning strategies, lecturing may promote passive, individual, and dependent
learning. If this is the metaphor
for learning and the strategy preferred by the instructor and it is opposite
that of some students, then there could be a strong disconnect in the learning
process, and students are likely to be unmotivated or disinterested.
By listening to our students’ voices and by taking their expectations
into account, we can potentially provide better tailored environments that
result in more effective learning.
Conclusion
Results of this investigation
display a breadth of student-generated metaphors that describe learning in all
its variety and complexity This
information allows us as their teachers to gain a better understanding of how
students value learning experiences. Abbott (2006) implies that challenging
students’ or teachers’ one-dimensional metaphoric views of good education and
so, ideally, of learning experiences is essential.
He notes the importance of avoiding being locked into any particular
metaphor for learning (e.g., conduit, container, journey, disease), whether
educator or student. By attending
to the wealth of student-generated metaphors and the themes they suggest,
perhaps we can better design and develop effective learning experiences.
The metaphors created by the students in this investigation provide a
starting point for that task.
Acknowledgements
This research was partially funded
by grants from the University of Wisconsin System Office of Professional and
Instructional Development and the American Association of Higher Education.
References
Abbott, C.
(2006, March 24).
The library effect.
Teaching Forum.
Retrieved May 11,
Annells, M. (1997). Grounded theory method, Pt. II: Options for users of the method. Nursing Inquiry, 4, 176-180.
Astin,
A. (1977).
Four
critical years:
Effects
of college on beliefs, attitudes and knowledge.
Aylor,
B., & Oppliger, P. (2003).
Out-of-class communication and student perceptions of instructor humor
orientation and socio-communicative style.
Communication Education, 52,
122-134.
Baxter, L. A., & Montgomery, B. M. (1996). Relating: Dialogues and dialectics.
Berger, C. R., & Calabrese, R. J. (1975). Some explorations in initial interaction and beyond: Toward a developmental theory of interpersonal communication. Human Communication Research, 1, 99-112.
Black, M. (1962). Models and metaphors: Studies in language and philosophy.
Blunsdon, B., Reed, K., McNeil, N., & McEachern, S. (2003). Experiential learning in social science theory: An investigation of the relationship between student enjoyment and learning. Higher Education Research & Development, 22, 43-56.
Cameron, L. (2003). Metaphor in educational discourse.
Colbeck, C. L., Campbell, S. E., & Bjorklund, S. A. (2000). Grouping in the dark. Journal of Higher Education, 71, 60-83.
Doran, M. S., Sullivan, H. J., & Klein, J. D. (1993, November). Student performance and attitudes using peer collaboration in accounting. Paper presented at the meeting of the Association for Educational Communications and Technology,
Edelman, M. (1971). Politics as symbolic action: Mass arousal and quiescence.
Eisenberg, E. M. (1984). Ambiguity as strategy in organizational communication. Communication Monographs, 51, 227-242.
Eubanks, P. (1999). Conceptual metaphor as rhetorical response. Written Communication, 16, 171-199.
Fassinger, R. E. (2005). Paradigms, praxis, problems, and promise: Grounded Theory in counseling psychology research. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52, 156-166.
Felder, R. M., Felder, G. N., & Diets, E. J. (1998). A longitudinal study of engineering student performance and retention vs. comparisons with traditionally-taught students. Journal of Engineering Education, 87, 469-480.
Frand, J. L. (2000). The information-age mindset: Changes in students and implications for higher education. Educause, 35(5), 14-24.
Franke, W. (2000). Metaphor and the making of sense: The contemporary metaphor renaissance. Philosophy and Rhetoric, 33, 137-153.
Gossi, R., Jr. (1999). The power of metaphor in the age of electronic media.
Grasha, T. (1990). The naturalistic approach to learning styles. College Teaching, 38 (3), 106-113.
Grasha, A. F. (1996). Teaching with style: A practical guide to enhancing learning by understanding teaching and learning.
Hardcastle, B., Yamamoto, K., Parkay, F. W., & Chan, J. (1985). Metaphorical views of school: A cross-cultural comparison of college students. Teaching and Teacher Education, 1, 309-315.
Herbster, D., & Hannula, J. J. (1992, February). Cooperative learning in the teacher preparation course. Paper presented at the meeting of the Association of Teacher Educators,
Howe, N. & Strauss, W. (2000). Millennials rising: The next great generation.
Hufford, R. (1966). The dimensions of an idea: Ambiguity defined. Today’s Speech, 14, 4-8.
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (1981). Effects of cooperative and individualistic learning experiences on interethnic interaction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 444-449.
Johnson, D. W., Johnson, R. T., & Taylor, B. (1993). Impact of cooperative and individualistic learning on high-ability students’ achievement, self-esteem, and social acceptance. Journal of Social Psychology, 133, 839-844.
Jones, A. C. (2008). The effects of out-of-class support on student satisfaction and motivation to learn. Communication Education, 57, 373-388.
Jorgensen-Earp, C. R., & Staton, A. Q. (1993). Student metaphors for the college freshman experience. Communication Education, 42, 123-141.
Kuh, G.
D. (1995). The other curriculum: Out-of-class experiences associated with
student learning and personal development.
The Journal of Higher Education,
66, 123-155.
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by.
Lancaster, L. C. & Stillman, D. (2002). When generations collide: Who they are, why they clash, how to solve the generational puzzle at work.
Lattin, B. D., Kerssen-Griep, J., & Thede, J.
(2002). Metaphors and motivation: Understanding college students’ learning experiences at four types of schools. College & University Journal, 78, 21-27.
Linkon, S. (2000). Students’ perspectives on interdisciplinary learning. In P. Hutchings (Ed.), Opening lines: Approaches to the scholarship of teaching and learning (pp. 63-71).
McCroskey, J. C.,
McMillan, J. J., & Cheney, G. (1996). The student as consumer: The implications and limitations of a metaphor. Communication Education, 45, 1-15.
Milem,
J. F., & Berger, J. B. (1997). A modified model of college student persistence:
Exploring the relationship between Astin’s theory of involvement and Tinto’s
theory of student departure.
Journal
of College Student Development,
38, 387-400.
Miller, J. E., Wilkes, J., Cheetham, R. D., & Goodwin, L. (1993). Tradeoffs in student satisfaction: Is the “perfect” course an illusion? Journal on Excellence in College Teaching, 4, 27-47.
Mills, J., Ayre, M., Hands, D., & Carden, P. (2005). Learning about learning styles: Can it improve engineering education? MountainRise, 2 (1). Accessed
Morehead, J. W., & Shedd, P. J. (1996). Student interviews: A vital role in the scholarship of teaching. Innovative Higher Education, 20, 261-269.
Nadler,
M. K., & Nadler L. B. (2001). The roles of sex, empathy, and credibility in
out-of-class communication between faculty and students.
Women’s
Studies in Communication,
24, 241-262.
Olson, K. (2001). Ambiguity. In T. O. Sloane (Ed.), Encyclopedia of rhetoric (pp. 21-25).
Ortony, A. (1975). Why metaphors are necessary and not just nice. Educational Theory, 25, 45-53.
Parry, K. W. (1998). Grounded theory and social process: A new direction for leadership research. Leadership Quarterly, 9, 85-106.
Pascale, R. T., & Athos, A. G. (1981). The art of Japanese management.
Pascarella, E. (1980). Student-faculty informal contact and college outcomes. Review of Educational Research, 50, 545-595.
Pascarella, E. T., & Terensini, P. T. (1991). How college affects students: Findings and insights from twenty years of research
.
Richards,
Salvatori, M. R., & Donahue, P. (2005). The elements (and pleasures) of difficulty.
Shulman, L. S. (2000). Inventing the future. In P. Hutchings (Ed.), Opening lines: Approaches to the scholarship of teaching and learning (pp. 95-105).
Shulman, L. S. (1999, July/August). Taking learning seriously. Change, 11-17.
Slavin, R. E. (1991). Synthesis of research on cooperative learning. Educational Leadership, 48, 71-82.
Smith, K. K., & Berg, D. N. (1987). Paradoxes of group life.
Spady, W. (1970). Dropouts from higher education: An interdisciplinary review and synthesis. Interchange, 1, 64-85
.
Strauss, A. L., & Corbin, J. (1990). Basics of qualitative research: Grounded theory procedures and techniques.
Strauss, A. L., & Corbin, J. (1998). Basics of qualitative research: Techniques and procedures for developing grounded theory (2nd ed.).
Taylor, W. (Ed.). (1984). Metaphors of education.
Theophilides, C., & Terensini, P. T. (1981). The relation between nonclassroom contact
with faculty and students’ perceptions of instructional quality. Research in Higher Education, 15, 255-269.
Tlusty, R., McIntyre, S., & Eierman, R. (1993, April). Cooperative learning in a college chemistry course. Paper presented at meeting of the American Educational Research Association,
Terensini, P. T., Pascarella, E. T., & Blimling, G. S. (1996). Students’ out-of-class experiences and their influence on learning and cognitive development: A literature review. Journal of College Student Development, 37, 149-162.
Weick, K. E. (1979). The social psychology of organising (2nd ed.).
Witt, P. L., Wheeless, L. R., & Allen, M. (2006). The relationship between teacher immediacy and student learning: A meta-analysis. In B. M. Gayle, R. W. Preiss, N. Burrell, & M. Allen (Eds.), Classroom communication and instructional processes: Advances through meta-analysis (pp. 149-168).